Touch at a Distance

Episode Summary

Episode Title: Touch at a Distance - Sound can be thought of as a form of touch at a distance. When sound waves enter our ears, they cause vibrations that are transmitted to our brain through electrical signals. - Tone languages like Mandarin Chinese utilize pitch to convey meaning. Studies show that young tone language speakers are much more likely to have perfect pitch compared to non-tone language speakers. This suggests tone languages may enhance musical abilities. - There appear to be universal melodies used by parents when speaking to infants across languages - rising pitch for praise, falling pitch for prohibition, high pitch to call attention. This implies an innate musicality in parent-child vocal communication. - In 1913, Stravinsky's dissonant Rite of Spring caused riots at its premier. But one year later it was well received. This demonstrates the brain's ability to adjust to challenging new music over time by learning to find order in disorder. - The transition of sound waves into electrical signals and dopamine in the brain helps explain music's power to make us feel euphoria or discomfort. As our brains decode music, we can learn to love even highly dissonant pieces.

Episode Show Notes

In this episode from 2007, we take you on a tour of language, music, and the properties of sound. We look at what sound does to our bodies, our brains, our feelings… and we go back to the reason we at Radiolab tell you stories the way we do. 

First, we look at Diana Deutsch’s work on language and music, and how certain languages seem to promote musicality in humans. Then we meet Psychologist Anne Fernald and listen to parents as they talk to their babies across languages and cultures. Last, we go to 1913 Paris and sneak into the premiere of Igor Stravinsky’s score of The Rite of Spring

Check out Diana Deutsch's 'Audio Illusions' here (https://deutsch.ucsd.edu/psychology/pages.php?i=201). 

Our newsletter comes out every Wednesday. It includes short essays, recommendations, and details about other ways to interact with the show. Sign up (https://radiolab.org/newsletter)!

Radiolab is supported by listeners like you. Support Radiolab by becoming a member of The Lab (https://members.radiolab.org/) today.

Follow our show on Instagram, Twitter and Facebook @radiolab, and share your thoughts with us by emailing radiolab@wnyc.orgCorrection: An earlier version of this piece incorrectly stated the dates of two performances of “Rite of Spring” and the time that passed between them. The performance that inspired rioting occurred on May 29th, 1913. The second performance that we discussed occurred in April of 1914. The audio has been adjusted to reflect this fact.Correction: An earlier version of this piece incorrectly stated that the “Rite of Spring” was used in the movie “Fantasia” during the part that featured mushrooms. It was in fact used during the part that featured dinosaurs. The audio has been adjusted to reflect this fact.

Leadership support for Radiolab’s science programming is provided by the Gordon and Betty Moore Foundation, Science Sandbox, a Simons Foundation Initiative, and the John Templeton Foundation. Foundational support for Radiolab was provided by the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation.

Episode Transcript

SPEAKER_18: Radiolab is supported by Apple Card. Apple Card has a cash-back rewards program unlike other credit cards. You earn unlimited daily cash on every purchase, receive it daily, and can grow it at 4.15 annual percentage yield when you open a savings account. Apply for Apple Card in the Wallet app on iPhone. Apple Card subject to credit approval. Savings is available to Apple Card owners subject to eligibility requirements. Savings accounts provided by Goldman Sachs Bank USA. Remember FDIC terms apply. SPEAKER_12: Listener supported. WNYC Studios. SPEAKER_13: This week on The New Yorker Radio Hour, the novelist Jennifer Egan on how we could end the enormous problem of homelessness if we had the will to do it. That's The New Yorker Radio Hour. Listen wherever you get your podcasts. SPEAKER_18: Hey, it's Lulu. To start today, I want to play you a sentence that I heard 20 years ago that changed the course of my life. SPEAKER_09: Sound is kind of touch at a distance. SPEAKER_18: I was 21 years old, living in Queens, working at a coffee shop, more than a little lonely, unsure what I was doing with my life. And boom. The sound is touch at a distance. I didn't know it yet, but it was this brand new show called Radio Lab that was doing something with sound and layering and music that almost no one was yet bringing to journalism. That episode would then go on to do this very meta thing of explaining how the words that were being spoken right then were just waves of vibrating air traveling across space and SPEAKER_18: time into my eardrum, which vibrates a few very small bones. SPEAKER_04: And the little bones transmit the vibration into this salty sea where the hairs are. And the hair cells are literally bent by a wave. They bend like trees in a breeze. And when these hair cells bend, charged molecules flood inwards and activate the cell. SPEAKER_18: And it was like, I felt it. This stranger's voice had just made contact, literal contact with me. And in that moment, I knew that was my path. I wanted to get into that world and use that medium to do that thing. Twenty years later, I still feel so lucky to be part of this show where every week we send out sounds and voices. SPEAKER_18: And experiences and emotions that actually touch you. SPEAKER_18: And look, I think in a moment like right now, when so many of us feel divided across space, across geography, across ideology, I think sounds potential to sneak in and connect. I think it really matters. But the truth is, and the reason I am here yammering about all this is that it's a really dire time in public media right now. And to be able to keep bringing you stories like this that are packed lovingly with sounds, sounds that we hope can help you not just understand a story, but feel it. We really need your support. We've run the numbers and found that less than 2% of you support Radiolab. And if we could get that number up just a hair, just a little bit, it would make a huge difference. The best way to do that is to join our membership program, The Lab, where you commit to tossing in just a few bucks a month. It is super easy to sign up. To check that out, you just go to radiolab.org slash join. And to entice you, we just made a brand new t-shirt that every single new member will get this month. And it has a great design. It is a reference to the episode you're about to hear. So more on that in a little bit. And if you join, you also get all kinds of other perks, special event invitations, extra interviews you can listen to. But really it is just about supporting the show, keeping us going. So again, if you want to check out what the t-shirt looks like, see how easy it is to join a few bucks, like a coffee a month, you can just go to radiolab.org slash join and pick the amount that's right for you. All right, thank you for listening to all that. And that's all I've got to say. I'm going to end this one with today's episode, which is actually that very episode that I heard 20 years ago. I hope you enjoy. SPEAKER_06: Wait, you're listening. SPEAKER_19: Okay. All right. You're listening to Radiolab. SPEAKER_14: Radiolab. SPEAKER_19: From WNYC. Rewind. I'll continue. SPEAKER_18: We're here to report the first large scale t-shirt. It's a little bit of a SPEAKER_10: large scale study comparing the prevalence of absolute pitch in two normal populations. SPEAKER_02: This is Professor Diana Deutsch. Diana Deutsch. SPEAKER_10: Well, yeah, I'm going to turn down my headphone level. And I'm a professor of psychology at the University of California, San Diego. Can you still hear me, Diana? SPEAKER_02: Okay. Hello? Diana studies sound, how humans perceive sound. She's a scientist, she has a lab, but every so often she will also release CDs. Right. These CDs of audio demonstrations that she uses in her research. And that's why we called, because it was in the production of her second CD that she stumbled onto the weirdest phenomenon. SPEAKER_10: Well, I'll tell you what happened is that when you do post-production, as you know, of speech, you loop things, loop things, loop things so that you can zero in on P's, P's that sound too loud, you need to un-pop, or S's that sound too sharp, and so on. So you put things on loops in order to fine tune the way the speech sounds. So I had this particular phrase on a loop and forgot about it. What phrase was this? It's a phrase that occurs at the beginning of the CD in which I say, the sounds as they appear to you are not only different from those that are really present, but they sometimes behave so strangely as to seem quite impossible. As to seem quite impossible. Now I had sometimes behaved so strangely looped. The sounds as they appear to you are not only different from those that are really present, but they sometimes behave so strangely, sometimes behave so strangely, just those few words, sometimes behave so strangely, and forgot about it, sometimes behave so strangely, sometimes behave so strangely. So here's what happened. SPEAKER_02: Diana leaves her studio. She closes the door, goes into the kitchen to make some tea. All the while this loop is whirring away in the background. As she's sipping her tea, she thinks, is someone singing? SPEAKER_10: Who's singing? I heard what sounded like song in the background. She realized, wait a second, that's not singing. SPEAKER_02: That's me talking. SPEAKER_10: That very phrase. Strangely, but at this point, sometimes behave so strange to be sung rather than spoken. Strangely sometimes behave so strangely. This is sometimes behave so strangely, right? You still hear the words, but they're sung words rather than spoken words. SPEAKER_02: It's weird. It just switches at a certain point. Three or four repetitions in, it's going, it's going, and then pow, it becomes music. And then now none of us can get it out of our head. The whole office is like, sometimes behave so strangely, sometimes behave so strangely. SPEAKER_10: And you know what? You do this demo and then you go back to the original sentence. It sounds like, you know, speech to begin with. And when you come to that very phrase, I seem to be bursting into song. The sounds as they appear to you are not only different from those that are really present, but they sometimes behave so strangely as to seem quite impossible. I have to say this can continue for months and months. It's sort of like your brain gets altered for that particular phrase and it continues to sound like singing for a very, very long time. SPEAKER_10: All right, so here we have just one small indication that music is, well, it behaves SPEAKER_02: very strangely. I mean, think about this. We started with some basic speech, repeated it a few times, somewhere along the way it leapt into song. SPEAKER_02: How did it change like that? And if that's all it takes to turn something into music, then what exactly is music really? SPEAKER_02: This is Radiolab. Today's program is about answering that question, or trying to in any case. I'm Jad Abumrad here with me. SPEAKER_07: This is Robert Kralwich, my partner in crime. SPEAKER_02: It's a little hard to get out of your head. I know. It is really weird. I know, I know. What are we doing? We are going to try. And we will probably fail. SPEAKER_06: Yes, we will fail. We will make an earnest effort to try to find the ingredients of music, both its basis in languages, basis in physics, its basis in your brain. We'll look everywhere we can, software, trying to find out what music is made of and why it touches us so intimately. SPEAKER_02: And touches us sometimes not in a good way. If you've ever had this experience of going to a concert, hearing some music, and it just made you upset, for some reason, like irrationally upset, almost like you wanted to hurt someone. If that rings a bell, there's a segment later in the show you will not want to miss. This is Radiolab. Stick around. SPEAKER_07: Sometimes they behave so strangely. SPEAKER_02: Alright, shall we start? Sure. Well, first, thanks to the LaGuardia High School chorus and Robert Apostle, they were the voices you just heard. We'll hear more of them later. SPEAKER_06: So let's explore a little bit more closely this connection between language and music. You think of them as separate. The thing is, they're really closely related, says neuroscientist Mark Jude Trammell. SPEAKER_17: When we speak, we sing. You know how to use the pitch of your voice to convey emotion and meaning. Like, I went to the store just because I raised the pitch, the note, if you will, you interpret that as an interrogative, a monotonic speech, you know, talking at the same rate and rhythm in the same pitch and loudness. I mean, that is not how humans talk. SPEAKER_02: But humans talk in all kinds of different ways, in different languages. Each language has its own musical personality. German is different than French, is different than Swahili. And if you look at those differences closely, there are all kinds of things we can learn about music. Take Diana Deutsch. Okay. She's recently been looking at tone languages. Just published her results, and the results are startling. Diana, before we start, what exactly is a tone language? SPEAKER_10: Okay. In tone languages, words take on different meanings depending upon the tones in which they are enunciated. For example, Mandarin has four tones and the word ma in Mandarin means mother in the first tone, hemp in the second tone, horse in the third tone, and a reproach in the fourth tone. Could you say them? The same... would you like me to? Yeah, could you demonstrate? Oh, I thought you were... I thought you were... Well, you know, I have them on CD, but I'd rather hear you say them. SPEAKER_02: SPEAKER_10: Well, okay, so excuse my bad pronunciation, but I'll try. Ma means mother. Ma means hemp. Ma means horse. And ma is a reproach. SPEAKER_02: So conceivably, if you screwed up the tones, you could call your mom a horse. SPEAKER_10: Yes, indeed. In fact, there are quite a lot of jokes where Westerners who don't speak the tones right say terrible things. SPEAKER_15: You have to be very careful. See, this is the basic difference. SPEAKER_02: In English, we don't really worry about pitch. We can say our words up here, or down here, or glisten up, or bend it down. It's all the same. Not so with tone languages. In any case, this is where it gets interesting. One day, Diana is working with some Mandarin speakers and she notices something. There were these words, these words that they would say where they would all hit precisely the same note with their voices, not just close to one another either. Totally precisely and consistently the same pitch. Even on different days. In fact, would you like me to play for you one person reciting a list of 12 Mandarin SPEAKER_10: tones on two different days? Yeah, definitely. Um, first you have the first word spoken on day one, followed by the same word spoken on day two, then you have the second word spoken on day one, followed by the same word spoken day two, and so on. And that way you can see the consistency. It's going to appear as though the words are being repeated immediately. But in fact, the repetitions occur on entirely different days. SPEAKER_02: So each of those word pairs came out of the mouth of one person separated by like 24 hours? Oh, much more than that. SPEAKER_10: Something like a week. Really? And it was a remarkable consistency. Well, that'd be like us saying the word mom always at this note right here. SPEAKER_02: Mom, mom, mom. SPEAKER_10: Well, I concluded that basically this was a form of perfect pitch. SPEAKER_06: I've never quite understood what perfect pitch is, to be honest. You don't know what that is? No. Should I? I mean, I know I should. SPEAKER_02: Well, it's... Just whisper it to me. As a musician growing up, perfect pitch is like the... it's like the thing. It's like the thing you wish you had. None of us have. Basically it's like having a tuning fork in your brain. SPEAKER_02: Here, I'll give you an analogy. Yeah. Okay, you see this coffee cup I'm holding? Yeah. What color is it? Brown. And you knew that how? Through my eyes. Right. So you need me to put this brown coffee cup next to my blue jeans. No, I didn't. In order to see the brown. I mean, it's absolute brown. It's absolute brown. Perfect pitch, people have that with pitch. They hear a pitch, they know exactly what note it is. The rest of us have to run to the piano. So if they hear a ding from an elevator, can they name the note? SPEAKER_02: Yeah, that's exactly it. Anything with a pitch, like a horn honk, they could tell you that horn is an F, or those church bells. They're alternating between B flat and B. And if the faucet were dripping, they'd say, they could say, that faucet is dripping and it's D sharp. They don't even have to think about it. They just know. SPEAKER_10: It used to be that the note names would jump out at me. Diana Deutsch is actually one of these lucky people. SPEAKER_10: To the extent that it would even be a nuisance. SPEAKER_06: And why is that good? SPEAKER_02: Well, it's really rare. It only happens like once every 10,000 people here in America or Europe. Yeah, but so does turning your tongue into a U. And of the people who have it? Yeah. Well, let's see, how should I say this? If you look in your music history textbooks, you will see that every famous composer, like the really big ones. SPEAKER_10: Like Mozart. And Bach. SPEAKER_08: And Beethoven. They all had it. SPEAKER_07: Oh, really? SPEAKER_08: Mendelssohn, the list goes on and on. SPEAKER_02: So if you have perfect pitch, on some level, you are closer to them. SPEAKER_02: You've got the gift. Let's get back to Diana Deutsch. Okay. Okay, let's talk about your latest experiment. That's the one I'm really interested in. Okay, so you compare Chinese kids to American kids to see who has perfect pitch more. So explain how this works. You had a group of Chinese music students, a group of American music students at the Eastman School of Music here in New York. You play them a bunch of notes, I imagine, in a room and ask them to guess what those notes were. Right. Now how did that work exactly? SPEAKER_10: Well, the test consisted of piano tones, which began on the C below middle C, that's this note, and extended up three octaves all the way up to that note. SPEAKER_02: That's a big range. SPEAKER_10: Yeah, 36 notes. Can you demonstrate? SPEAKER_02: Sure, yes. SPEAKER_10: Here are six tones such as were given in the test. SPEAKER_02: So you would have played those notes to both sets of kids and asked them to name the notes without going to the piano. What were the notes really? SPEAKER_10: What these notes were D, E, G sharp, C sharp, D sharp, and G. SPEAKER_02: What were the results? SPEAKER_10: Well, it turns out the Chinese group far outperformed the Eastman group. Of those students who started musical training at ages four and five, 74% of the Chinese group show perfect pitch, but 14% of the US non-tonal would speak. Wow, 74%? SPEAKER_10: The Beijing group was nine times roughly more likely to show perfect pitch than the American English speaking American group. Jesus, that's a staggering difference. And it's your hunch that the difference is because they speak a tone language? SPEAKER_10: That's my hunch. I mean, it's known that in the first year of life, say from age six months up to a little SPEAKER_10: past a year, infants learn features of their native language. This is a very, very important stage. Let's suppose that tone and the absolute pitch of tones is a feature which is potentially available to anyone. Babies who are exposed only to an international language such as English are not given the opportunity to acquire tones. Then they're going to be at a real disadvantage when they come later on to learn to take music lessons. SPEAKER_02: So you think that as they're learning their language, which includes inherently music to some degree, they are essentially learning two languages as they learn one. Is that right? SPEAKER_10: As a matter of fact, if you take the first tone, it's a flat tone. It's really sung. Compared with English speech, it's really more like song. That's always been sort of the stereotype of the Chinese language is it's very sort SPEAKER_02: of singsongy. SPEAKER_15: Yes. For example, the third tone in Mandarin, is sort of like a J type pattern. SPEAKER_10: The second tone, which is a gentle upward gliss, the fourth tone, which is a rapid downward gliss. I mean, these are all kind of musical relationships. Given the evidence on absolute pitch, one could speculate further and say, well, maybe other features of music are also enhanced for individuals who start off learning tone language. SPEAKER_02: So here's my big question. Could this explain the experience that I had? I think a lot of people have this experience when they're taking music lessons and playing little piddly pieces like Frere Jacques. And here are these Chinese girls, right? Who are playing Rachmaninoff. You know, they're brilliant. SPEAKER_02: Is this why? SPEAKER_10: Well, I think it's a viable hypothesis. I mean, evidently it could be something else. There could be something else going on. Like what? I mean, one could argue that instead it might be genetic and so on. But that's such a boring theory. SPEAKER_10: It's a boring theory. And furthermore, we don't have to assume that knowing what we do about exposure to tone language in very early childhood as a... It's just not fair. And I think we can look at it another way around. SPEAKER_10: Here we have a faculty that had been thought to be confined to a few rare individuals who are just extraordinarily gifted. That might in fact be available to any individual provided they're given the right exposure at a critical period. SPEAKER_10: And that raises the question of what other sorts of abilities could be brought out if we only knew just what to do. There may be much more human potential than we had realized. SPEAKER_02: Diana Deutsch is a professor of music psychology at the University of San Diego. Music psychology? Music psychology. And as I mentioned earlier, she's also the releaser of two CDs. Yes, I forgot. Yes, two CDs. One's called Musical Illusions and Paradoxes, and the other one is called Phantom Words and Other Curiosities. What would she put on a CD exactly? She puts these little audio pieces that she uses in her research, the stuff I guess that she will play to subjects as she tests them. And she puts these on CDs because they're kind of fun to listen to. This is like an ear test or... Yeah, sort of. We've actually put a couple on our website. Well, what do they sound like? Just gives a little sample. All right, I'll do some samples. There is the chromatic illusion. SPEAKER_02: Kind of has a carnival feel to it. There's also the cambiata illusion. Oh, the cambiata illusion. And of course, the phantom word experiments. Ah, the phantom word experiment. SPEAKER_02: None of those pieces are going to make any kind of sense unless you visit our website, radiolab.org, where all will be explained. SPEAKER_07: Sometimes they behave so strangely. Sometimes they behave so strangely. Sometimes they behave so strangely. SPEAKER_18: Sometimes they behave so strangely. Lulu here. More Radiolab in a moment. We will hear that phrase. Sound is touch at a distance. Learn about what that means. Learn about how it can be such a powerful sound. It will make old French ladies throw things. But first, I wanted to just say that that song is still stuck in my head 20 years later. It has been one of the hookiest earworms I've ever encountered in my life. And I have finally used my powers as host to get it on a t-shirt. That's right. We just made a brand new t-shirt that says, sometimes behaves so strangely. I think that phrase is kind of a motto for what we are doing here. The magic of sound, the power of sound. And you know, if you wear it, it might be talking about how sound works or it might be talking about the person inside the shirt who sometimes behaves so strangely. Anyway, it's really cool. It comes in four different colors. I'm so excited about it. And it can be yours if you join the lab this month and support us. So this is your moment. Get a great shirt. Support our little operation here of Sonic investigation and tomfoolery. It would mean so much. The way you can do that, you can check out the shirt and consider joining. You just go to radiolab.org slash join. radiolab.org slash join. Join the party. Thanks. SPEAKER_16: More Radiolab in a moment. Hi, Rebecca Murray here from Mount Vernon, Washington. I'm a member of Radiolab's exclusive membership program, The Lab. My membership provides Radiolab with a steady source of funding so the team can continue to tell stories about our crazy world. And I get access to exclusive live events and bonus content. Join me in supporting the show. We love. Sign up at radiolab.org slash join. SPEAKER_18: Radiolab is supported by Capital One with no fees or minimums. Banking with Capital One is the easiest decision in the history of decisions. Even easier than deciding to listen to another episode of your favorite podcast. And with no overdraft fees, is it even a decision? That's banking reimagined. What's in your wallet? Terms apply. Visit Capital One dot com slash bank Capital One N.A. member FDIC. Radiolab is supported by Apple Card. Apple Card has a cash back rewards program unlike other credit cards. You earn unlimited daily cash on every purchase, receive it daily and can grow it at four point one five annual percentage yield. When you open a savings account, apply for Apple Card in the wallet app on iPhone. Apple Card subject to credit approval savings is available to Apple Card owners subject to eligibility requirements, savings accounts provided by Goldman Sachs Bank USA member FDIC. Terms apply. After but her emails became shorthand in 2016 for the media's deep focus on Hillary Clinton's SPEAKER_05: server hygiene at the expense of policy issues, is history repeating itself? SPEAKER_12: You can almost see an equation again, I would say, led by the Times in Biden being old, with Donald Trump being under dozens of felony indictments. SPEAKER_05: Listen to On the Media from WNYC. Find On the Media wherever you get your podcasts. SPEAKER_02: This is Radiolab. I'm Chad Abumrad. And I'm Robert Krelwich. Our program today is about music. What it is, how it works. And what we want to do next is we want to we want to stay on the subject, but we're SPEAKER_06: going to explore this a little more deeply. Take a closer look at the connection between language and music. We're going to add touch, touch, and that will take us to the ear, the ear, and then into the brain, the brain, and then to the big question, the really big question, why does music or how does music become a feeling the feeling? Why do we get such deep pleasure or deep pain? We will have pain coming up to all simply because of air pressing against your ear. All right. Oh, we're first. All right. So there's a psychology professor I want you to meet at Stanford who directs the Center for Infant Studies there. Anne Fernolds is her name. And she got it into her head that there is a kind of deep universal music inside language. And she discovered it actually at a hospital. The Max Planck Institute in Munich has an obstetrics unit, which is very popular among expectant mothers. These mothers came from the wards of this German hospital. SPEAKER_09: And so they were Turkish. They were Greek. They were Sicilian. They were the so-called guest workers in the German society. Of course, I didn't understand a word of what they spoke. As soon as they put the baby down and no longer had the physical contact, bodily contact with the child, they started to sing almost in one language after another. I book you one the babies. SPEAKER_09: Dutch. I heard these, I heard them use these melodies. Russian. Russian. No. To reach the child, to remain in touch with the baby. Yiddish. Yiddish. So the next day I brought my tape recorder. SPEAKER_06: Anne Fernolds took her tape recorder from that hospital and traveled all over the world recording parents as they talked to their very little babies. And it didn't matter whether the parents spoke a romance language or a tone language. Everywhere she went, below the words she heard consistently the same melodies. For example, I'll start with approval. SPEAKER_06: When a parent wanted to praise a child, we would ask the parents to show the baby they SPEAKER_09: were happy. Good boy. Now you got it. Just using their voice, show them you're happy with that. SPEAKER_05: Das. Achateri gari, gedara gari. Hindi. SPEAKER_06: Daddy. SPEAKER_07: I'm in sab. Qua lo kavalo. Portuguese. SPEAKER_06: Ey, si poi, si so kavalo. SPEAKER_15: And what these things had in common was that the melody was a kind of a rise, fall. SPEAKER_09: Ma'am. Good girl. Good girl. SPEAKER_19: You got it. Yeah. Good girl, sweetie. SPEAKER_06: So it doesn't matter what words the parents are saying. It's always really about this melody. SPEAKER_02: And why that particular melody? All she knows is it works. SPEAKER_09: There's something about this melody that keeps a child doing something. SPEAKER_06: There are, she says, other categories, she discovered. Now with a prohibition, in contrast, your goal is to stop the child from doing something. SPEAKER_09: The category that says stop. Quite a different melody. It's short. Stop. It's sharp. Stop. In musical terms, it's staccato. SPEAKER_06: There is the category of look. Pay attention to that. SPEAKER_09: Teachers frequently use rising pitch. Look, sweetie. They frequently use higher pitch. A unicorn. SPEAKER_06: A unicorn. So far, Anne Fernald has found four universal melodies that praise, that stop, that call attention and, of course, the melody that comforts. And while this may seem obvious to you if you think about it, this is music that is understood by infants who are just new in the world. But we all know what it means. We all know these songs. SPEAKER_09: We're used to thinking of sounds as being about something. Speech is always about something. But it feels to me more like touch. Touch isn't about something. If you whack me on the arm in a sudden sharp way, I'm going to be startled. Or a gentle touch has a different effect. And I think, you know, actually, sound is kind of touch at a distance. SPEAKER_06: I was Anne Fernald, director of the Center for Infant Studies at Stanford. And when Anne says, SPEAKER_09: Sound is more like touch. SPEAKER_06: That turns out to be literally the case. This is something I learned from a friend of mine, Jonah Lehrer. My name's Jonah Lehrer. SPEAKER_04: Who at this very moment is working on a book. An upcoming book on art and science, on the connection between art and science. SPEAKER_18: Lulu here real quick, a few years after this aired, serious problems with Jonah Lehrer's journalism would come to light. Everything in this episode has been fact checked and stands. SPEAKER_06: Anne says Jonah, thinking about sound as a touch. More like touch. I asked him, how does sound get into or touch your brain? SPEAKER_06: Take us on that journey. SPEAKER_04: It's just waves of vibrating air. It's just your voice. SPEAKER_04: Beginning your voice box compresses air, and that air travels through space and time. Into my ear. A little tunnel. Waves of diffuse vibrating air, focused and channeled. Into my eardrum. Which vibrates a few very small bones. And the little bones transmit the vibration into this salty sea. Where the hairs are. And the hair cells are fastening. The hair cells become active when they are literally bent by a wave. They bend like trees in a breeze. And when these hair cells bend, charged molecules flood inwards. SPEAKER_03: And activate the cells. SPEAKER_06: So the sound triggers the bones. The bones disturbs the fluid. The fluid rocks the hairs. Yes. And then the hairs set off essentially electricity? Yes. Huh. SPEAKER_04: That's the language of neurons. SPEAKER_06: All those changes from waves to bones to electricity, all those things were a trip on their way to being heard. It's only when the electricity finally forms a pattern in your brain, only when it's deep inside, that's when you hear something. SPEAKER_09: Feels to me. More like touch. Sound is kind of touch at a distance. SPEAKER_06: All right, now that we have gotten a sound, any sound, into our heads, let me ask you the next really big question. Why do some sounds, let's make it music, okay? Why does music make so many of us so often feel so strongly? Yeah, like in terms of what we were just listening to, like how does all that electricity from SPEAKER_02: the ear going up to the brain in the next millisecond become a feeling? Yeah. Well, let me introduce you to someone. Mark Trutremo. Actually, we heard from him earlier. He's a neuroscientist. I'm in the Department of Neurology at Harvard Medical School. SPEAKER_02: And Mark can at least begin to answer this question, this feeling question. He's done something really interesting. He's able to listen to the electricity as it pulls in the ear and shoots up this big fat nerve to the brain. SPEAKER_17: It's kind of a popping sound. SPEAKER_02: He can actually listen to that nerve, to the electricity. SPEAKER_17: Also faster than I'm able to do here with my fingers. SPEAKER_06: Is that the sound? That's what it sounds like. So you hear how that's... Wait, how do they get this sound? SPEAKER_02: I actually have no idea. I guess they sort of tap into the nerve. SPEAKER_06: This is the sound of sound entering a brain? SPEAKER_02: Yes, this is the sound of sound entering the brain as electricity, little pulses. And as you can hear, the electricity has a meter. SPEAKER_02: What Mark has discovered is that when the electricity entering your brain is even and regular... Is this regular? This is regular. SPEAKER_06: Hold on, wait. Yeah, that's regular. Right. When the meter of the electricity is regular and rhythmic, it will arrive in our mind and SPEAKER_02: be heard by us as a sound that we generally like. Like this. Nice sound. That, in music, is known as a perfect fifth. SPEAKER_17: The input's coming from a perfect fifth is very, very regular. SPEAKER_02: Like a metronome. However, and here's where it gets interesting, when the meter going from the ear to the brain is irregular, jagged, arrhythmic, unpredictable, strange... Wait, let me hear it. This is jagged? This is jagged. Wait, shh. SPEAKER_06: Oh, here it is jagged. Yeah, and what Mark has discovered is that when electrical impulses like that travel SPEAKER_02: from the ear to the brain, they will become heard by us, by our mind, as a sound that we generally don't like. Like that. Don't care for that one. That's a minor second. SPEAKER_17: The input's coming from a minor second is very, very chaotic. SPEAKER_06: Okay, so let me just sum up here what I think you're saying. If a sound entering my brain is disorderly and unexpected... Electrically speaking. Electrically speaking, then that would make me feel uncomfortable. Yeah. And if it comes in in a familiar and orderly way, that will make me feel comfortable. SPEAKER_02: There does seem to be a relationship between the kind of electricity a sound produces and how we feel about that sound. SPEAKER_06: Do they have like fancy names for this? Well that's a minor second. SPEAKER_02: That thing you just heard. SPEAKER_06: But do the scientists have names for pleasant and unpleasant? SPEAKER_02: Consonant is pleasant. Consonant is unpleasant. That's not a science name, that's a music name. Oh, okay. And these are fixed positions in your ear. SPEAKER_06: Well, it may be fixed for scientists, but you know, maybe, let me just propose this to you, that what people find pleasing and what people find painful is malleable. I'll tell you why. I'm going to tell you a story now, a true story, and it involves a musician. SPEAKER_04: Igor Stravinsky, who is now considered to be one of the great composers of the 20th century, if not the most important composer of the 20th century. SPEAKER_06: That's Jonah Lehrer again. And Jonah tells the story of two concerts, one year apart, in the same city, the exact same piece of music. The audience that heard it first and the audience that heard it second heard totally different things. So let's begin. First, Jonah, how does this set the scene? This is May 1913. SPEAKER_04: It's a spring night? It's a balmy summer night. Black tie costumes, the women have their fedoras. This was evening clothes. Yeah, well, this was the Russian ballet. This was high art. SPEAKER_06: And the program said this is a concert about springtime, but as they settled into their seats, it turns out that what Stravinsky had in mind was not spring like honeybees. No, the spring Stravinsky had in mind was about change, about radical change, ritual murder. SPEAKER_04: And that's what the story of the play is. It's a pagan ritual where the end of the Virgin gets massacred. SPEAKER_04: But the music itself is fascinating. The beginning is this very charming bassoon. It's a classic Lithuanian folk tune, and it does sound like the earth is warming. And that lasts for about a minute. And then we get some tutti of flute. It's lovely. It's getting a little more disturbing. And then about three minutes into it, everything changes. There's just an earthquake. Stravinsky plays this chord. There's a great story that when Diaghilev, who's the head of the Ballet Russe, first heard this chord and Stravinsky was playing it on piano form, he asked Stravinsky, how long will it go on like that? And Stravinsky looked at him and said, to the end, my dear. And it literally does. That chord structures the music. It's one of the most difficult sounds you've ever heard. It is just the stereotype of dissonance. It is. It hurts you. SPEAKER_06: Well, what happened? SPEAKER_04: Well, after about three minutes, they rioted. They what? SPEAKER_08: They rioted. Meaning what? Like they screamed or threw? SPEAKER_04: They screamed. There was blood. Old ladies were hitting each other with canes. Why were old ladies? SPEAKER_06: Old ladies should have gone and hit Stravinsky with a cane. SPEAKER_04: But once they started screaming, Stravinsky ran backstage and by some accounts was crying. Maginski was off on the side of the stage screaming to his dancers to keep the beat. Quite the fiasco. And the question is why? SPEAKER_06: This is the feeling question. Why so much feeling about a piece of music? Why did they riot? You would think that they rioted because they were hot, because they didn't like those sounds, because they thought those dancers were making strange and odd gestures. Well, Jonah offers a different theory. SPEAKER_02: Let me put it this way. This riot has been talked about and written about for forever. But to the best of our knowledge, no one has ever tried to explain what happened that night through the lens of brain chemistry. Brain chemistry? Yeah. What music can do to a brain? You know, if you try to imagine yourself where all you've heard is Wagner and the great romanticism SPEAKER_04: of 19th century music. And then all of a sudden you get this. I mean, these are noises you've never heard before. SPEAKER_02: Now it's all very new, but scientists are beginning to figure out what happens in our brain when we hear noises we've never heard before, especially dissonant noises. SPEAKER_19: We find that chords, musical chords that are typically judged to be dissonant, elicit these wild fluctuations in brain activity. This is Jan Fishman. SPEAKER_02: He is a neuroscientist and he studies those wild fluctuations in the brain. On an area of the brain called the auditory cortex. Let's zoom into the auditory cortex for a moment because this is basically hearing central. And when you're listening to music, there are all kinds of neurons doing all kinds of things. One gang in particular that Jan is interested in. That's right. A gang that he suspects gets very agitated when it hears sounds like these. These neurons might be the new noise department because he thinks their job is to take every new strange unordered, unpredictable noise that comes into the brain and figure it out. SPEAKER_04: Find the pattern. There are groups of neurons whose sole job it is to turn that dissonant note, dissect it, take it apart and try to understand it. We are pattern searching animals. SPEAKER_19: And this is how Jan Fishman puts it. And so at the level of the auditory cortex, the brain has this daunting task of having to be able to disentangle this complex mixture of sounds. SPEAKER_02: Most of the time those neurons in the auditory cortex succeed in finding the pattern. SPEAKER_02: But every so often, maybe this was the case that night, they fail. SPEAKER_08: Okay, so Robert, imagine inside the brains, inside the heads of the people in the audience SPEAKER_02: listening to the Rite of Spring that night, were all of these neurons trying to make sense of the new sounds and failing. Not just failing once or twice, but over and over and over. SPEAKER_02: Yeah, because the Rite of Spring keeps being dissonant all the way through so they can SPEAKER_06: never get any rest. SPEAKER_02: And when those neurons fail, repeatedly, there are consequences. Chemical consequences. SPEAKER_04: What happens is our neurons squirt out a bit of dopamine. What does the dopamine do? Well, dopamine makes us feel. A little dopamine makes you feel happy. SPEAKER_02: That's why sex and drugs make you feel euphoric. But a little too much. SPEAKER_04: The night euphoria turns into, literally, schizophrenia. Really? Yes. I don't want to oversimplify schizophrenia in any way, shape, or form. But some of our most effective treatments for schizophrenia work by suppressing dopamine release in the brain. SPEAKER_06: So there's some kind of relationship. Too much dopamine has been shown clinically to make people feel crazy. SPEAKER_02: Yes. Maybe that's what happened that night on May 29th, 1913. SPEAKER_06: Music erupted, neurons... Neurons revolted. Right. Dopamine flooded through their brains. And people went mad. Literally mad. SPEAKER_06: Let's go to the second night. This piece comes back to Paris, does it not? Yes. How much later after the riot? SPEAKER_04: Uh, it's from May to March. Oh. So it's almost a year later. Yes. And this time it doesn't come with a ballet. This time it's just being performed as a work of music. So does anyone buy tickets? SPEAKER_04: Oh yeah, it's gonna sell out. It caused a few nights of violent riots. SPEAKER_06: Can you set up the situation now, the audience? Is it a different audience? Uh, I actually don't know if the audience is different. SPEAKER_06: But we can at least say that the audience is coming to it with a different set of information. Exactly. They've been warned. SPEAKER_04: So for the first time they can actually sit back and really try to pay attention to the notes. By being willing to listen, they could hear the orders and patterns that Stravinsky had hidden in this work. They were able to hear the music and find the orders hidden underneath this noise. SPEAKER_06: Was there a riot this time, the second round? SPEAKER_04: Oh no, quite the opposite. Stravinsky was a hero. They carried him out on their shoulders. SPEAKER_04: Really? Literally? He was... Literally, he was carried on their shoulders and the press was glowing. In one year? In one year. SPEAKER_02: In one year, Stravinsky had gone from villainous monster to hipster icon. To the extent that police had to escort him from the concert hall to keep him safe from adoring fans. And that was just the beginning. SPEAKER_04: The third story, if you wanted to tell a third story, would be it became children's music. It became Disney music in 1940. 27 years after Stravinsky had caused a violent bloody riot, he was negotiating with Mickey SPEAKER_02: Mouse over the rights to use his music in Fantasia. SPEAKER_06: Which Fantasia? Is it starring a hippopotamus in a little tutu? Is that the one? SPEAKER_02: Is it the mushrooms, Jonah? Yeah, I think it's the mushrooms, isn't it? Actually, we looked it up later. It was the part with the dinosaurs. SPEAKER_06: So how does this happen? How do you go so quickly from being the most outrageous thing that literally maddens people to a triumph to kids' music? Yes, I mean, the Rite of Spring is perfect evidence of the brain's astonishing plasticity. SPEAKER_04: See, this is the really cool part of it for me. SPEAKER_02: If you remember just one bit of science from this whole thing, remember this. Those neurons we met earlier? The ones with the little voices? SPEAKER_06: I like them, yes. SPEAKER_02: It turns out those neurons learn. And they learn fast. Because they're actually part of a larger network of brain cells with a very technical name. SPEAKER_04: The birefugal network. SPEAKER_02: And what this network does is it's always sort of monitoring, listening to the sounds that are coming into the brain and tuning those neurons to better hear those sounds. SPEAKER_06: We're trying to get the station on the radio, just getting it just right. SPEAKER_04: So our neurons literally adjust. Literally. We're talking the biochemical engineering sense. So if on that first night you just hear the Rite as pure noise all the way through from beginning to end, if you're listening, if you're letting your corticofugal network do its job, it can actually re-sculpt your brain and let you hear the patterns better as the symphony evolves. Is it fair to say that this is a sort of tug of war? SPEAKER_06: That an artist comes, creates something that is new and unpredictable and strange and maybe noisish at first hearing. And the artist is thrilled to be new in that way. And then the brain ruins it all slowly but surely by making it familiar. SPEAKER_04: Well the brain abhors the new. The brain constantly wants to assimilate every experience we've ever had into every other experience. And I think Stravinsky realized it was the purpose of the artist to challenge the brain, to break the brain out of its conservative cycle. SPEAKER_06: The astonishing thing to me is here you have an artist like Igor Stravinsky who comes to town intentionally trying to get people to sit in their seats and really listen to music. And the strategy he chooses is instead of pleasing them, he wants to put them in a little bit of discomfort or real pain even. And indeed they not only listen, they riot. But within a year, and this is the sad part to me, within a year it's easier to hear, suddenly it's pleasant, suddenly they like it. And suddenly Igor Stravinsky is robbed of his newness. Why is that sad for you? Well because it's kind of, I don't know, I never thought of the brain as the enemy of the artist before. Yeah, but I can give you a different interpretation on this. SPEAKER_02: Sure. I mean here comes a guy who offers up the most dissonant, stabbing, percussive, painful music that anyone had heard to that point. And we learn to love it. Doesn't that make you sort of feel like pride? No, no. I mean like our brains can decode anything. We learn to love it only because it's well made. SPEAKER_06: Yeah, but... It was just random car honks. I don't think you could really appreciate that. I disagree. SPEAKER_02: Or you think it would be like just... Have you heard the music that was written after Stravinsky? It's even worse than what you just did. Yeah, but my brain has never accommodated that. But some people love that stuff. And my only point is that if there are these like fixed poles in our ear between consonance and dissonance, which is how we started this whole thing, and now we end up learning that our brains can override that to such an astonishing degree, well then culture wins. Culture beats biology. That's true, but to me it's sad. SPEAKER_06: It's sad for the artist. SPEAKER_02: It's not sad for us. SPEAKER_06: It's sort of like the artists and the brains are in a kind of eternal struggle. SPEAKER_18: That'll do it for today. Additional production by Sara Khare, and we will throw some new sounds at you next week. SPEAKER_14: Radiolab was created by Jad Abumrad and is edited by Soren Wheeler. Lulu Miller and Lautem Nasser are our co-hosts. Dylan Keefe is our director of sound design. Our staff includes Simon Adler, Jeremy Blum, Becca Bressler, Rachel Cusick, Akati Faustertis, W. Harry Fortuna, David Gable, Maria Paz-Gutierrez, Sundu Nyanam Sambana, Matt Kielty, Annie McEwen, Alex Neeson, Sara Khare, Ana Roscoez-Paz, Alyssa Jung Perry, Sarah Sandback, Carrie-Anne Wack, Pat Walters, and Molly Webster. Our fact checkers are Diane Kelly, Emily Krieger, and Natalie Middleton. SPEAKER_01: Hi, this is Finn calling from Storrs, Connecticut. Leadership support for Radiolab science programming is provided by the Gordon and Betty Moore Foundation, Science Sandbox, the Simons Foundation Initiative, and the John Templeton Foundation. Foundational support for Radiolab was provided by the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation. SPEAKER_00: WNYC Studios is supported by On Being with Christa Tippett. I'm Christa Tippett of On Being, where we take up the big questions of meaning for this world now. In our new podcast season, we're going to have a different human conversation about AI and also the intelligence of our bodies, grief and joy, social creativity and poetry, and so much more. A conversation to live by every Thursday.